• Email Us: [email protected]
  • Contact Us: +1 718 874 1545
  • Skip to main content
  • Skip to primary sidebar

Medical Market Report

  • Home
  • All Reports
  • About Us
  • Contact Us

How The Public Decides When Scientists Disagree

August 8, 2023 by Deborah Bloomfield

Scientists, and many others, would love it if people would “trust the science” when faced with important public or personal decisions. However, only a tiny proportion of the population will have a deep knowledge of research on any topic, so most people most of the time have to rely on what scientists say about it. We can talk all we like about what people should do when scientists disagree, as they frequently do, but maybe looking at how people actually decide which scientists to trust would be helpful as well.

This is what Dr Branden Johnson of Decision Research and colleagues are doing. Their results start the process of revealing what might work when scientists and science communicators want the reality of a scientific debate to cut through the misinformation.

Advertisement

Johnson and colleagues took three live scientific debates and presented statements to 3,150 Americans to test what influenced them on which side to favor. Of these, two – the sea level rise from climate change and the effects of cannabis – are hot button political concerns linked to culture wars. The third, the composition of dark matter, is relatively free from enrolment in such conflicts, although Creationists occasionally try.

“Our goal here is to explore which cues people use to determine which side in a dispute among many scientists to believe,” the study’s authors write. People may come to some debates – the effects of cannabis being a particularly clear example – with a preexisting lean, but they’ll also be influenced by the way the scientists present their case and what they’re told about those scientists.

Participants in the study were presented with a few sentences of background about what scientists agree and disagree on in relation to the debate and were asked a series of questions. Some of these concerned how much they knew about the topic and cared about who was right, while others related to their views on science more broadly.

They were then shown a series of cues that might influence their position, such as: “About 75% of the scientists with expertise on this topic support Position A”; “The information supporting position B has on average been collected more recently with newer techniques,”; or “Scientists who support Position B average 7.5 more years of experience in doing this kind of research.”

Advertisement

After being shown several of these cues, participants were asked which side’s position they thought was right, and repeats of the initial questions. Further cues were then shown, and the questions asked again, allowing the researchers to test the relative power of different cues. 

Not surprisingly, cues on where most scientists were aligned, which side’s evidence came from more advanced techniques, and that independent teams had reached the same conclusions were all quite persuasive. Having the more experienced scientists also counted for quite a bit, even though some might see this as evidence of being behind the times. These results also matched those of a previous study by Johnson on the same topic with a different design.

On the other hand, participants were less swayed by learning a position was favored by scientists who graduated from more prestigious universities, or whose employer stood to gain from one position winning.

None of the cues had much effect on whether participants cared about who was right. Some did, however, increase mistrust for science, showing they need to be used with care.

Advertisement

Not surprisingly, cues were more influential in relation to dark matter – where participants were unlikely to have pre-existing positions – than on cannabis, with sea levels in between. More unexpectedly, factors like prior engagement with the topic and knowledge of science had no discernable impact on how influenced people were by the cues.

There’s a lot of anxiety about the sight of experts disagreeing in public and how that will affect those outside the field. “[T]he dispute may threaten belief in the value of science overall rather than just in the value of the specific science being disputed. A dispute may be even more threatening when among large groups of scientists on each side, rather than (say) one individual scientist versus another,” Johnson and colleagues write. They add, however, “Concealing disputes could be equally problematic.” Consequently, there has been considerable investigation of whether witnessing scientific disagreements makes people lose confidence in science.

The authors note that in contrast, the question of how people make up their minds between competing groups of scientists hasn’t been much studied: “More attention has been devoted to disputes between scientists on one side and nonscientists on the other,” they write. 

The study is published in Risk Analysis.

Deborah Bloomfield
Deborah Bloomfield

Related posts:

  1. Soccer – FIFA backs down on threat to fine Premier clubs who play South American players
  2. U.S. House passes abortion rights bill, outlook poor in Senate
  3. Two children killed in missile strikes on Yemen’s Marib – state news agency
  4. Study Reveals Which Humans Survived The Last Ice Age And Which Didn’t

Source Link: How The Public Decides When Scientists Disagree

Filed Under: News

Primary Sidebar

  • First-Ever Living Recipient Of A Pig-To-Human Liver Transplant Survived For 171 Days
  • 190-Million-Year-Old “Sword Dragon Of Dorset” Likely The World’s Most Complete Pliensbachian Reptile
  • Acting CDC Director Calls For Splitting Up MMR Shots – But There’s A Reason We Don’t Do That
  • New Species Of Tiny Poison Dart Frog With Stripy Back And Spotty Legs Loves Bamboo
  • Not A Canine, Nor A Feline: Four Incredibly Cute Fossa Pups Have Been Born At A Zoo
  • The Most “Pristine Star” In The Universe May Have Been Identified – Researchers Link It To Elusive “Population III” Stars
  • 78-Million-Year-Old Crater Reveals Asteroid Impacts Can Create Long-Lasting Habitats For Microbial Life
  • 24 Years Of NASA Satellite Data Suggest The World Is Getting Darker, And It’s Happening Faster In The North
  • Two Black Holes Circling Each Other Captured In Image For The Very First Time
  • Rapa Nui’s Famous Moai Statues Really “Walked” – Physics Confirms It
  • Could Dogs Be Taught To Talk With Language? This Lab Wants To Find Out
  • SETI Paper Responds To Claims Interstellar Object 3I/ATLAS Might Be An Alien Spacecraft
  • Rare Chance To See “Pink Meanie” Jellyfish With 20-Meter Tentacles Blooming Off Texas
  • Stranded Dolphins’ Brains Show Signs Of Alzheimer’s-Like Disease
  • Natural Sweetener Stevia Could Help Bolster Common Hair Loss Treatment
  • “Dig Deep, And Persevere”: Number 16, The World’s Longest-Lived Spider, Died Aged 43
  • IFLScience The Big Questions: What Is Time And How Do We Measure It?
  • Marty Goddard: The History Of The Sexual Assault Kit
  • What’s Really Lurking In The Deep Dark Waters Of Loch Ness?
  • Another Comet 3I/ATLAS Record Got Us Asking: How Do We Know An Object Is Interstellar?
  • Business
  • Health
  • News
  • Science
  • Technology
  • +1 718 874 1545
  • +91 78878 22626
  • [email protected]
Office Address
Prudour Pvt. Ltd. 420 Lexington Avenue Suite 300 New York City, NY 10170.

Powered by Prudour Network

Copyrights © 2025 · Medical Market Report. All Rights Reserved.

Go to mobile version